Research

Cognitive neuroethology in Drosophila

The Maimon lab aims to understand how brains internally compute and store the value of quantitative variables–like heading angles, spatial distances, time intervals, and event probabilities–and then use these variables to guide behavior. By studying this topic in Drosophila, a classic genetic system, a central goal of the lab is to better understand how molecules, through their effect on cellular electrophysiology, impact memory and cognition.

Since the 1960’s, it has been widely accepted that explaining animal behavior requires more than defining stimulus-response relationships. Rather, cognitive processes like spatial attention, reward expectation, sensory prediction, and the construction of internal spatial maps, seem to serve a fundamental role in modifying these relationships. Yet, conceptual frameworks for thinking about such internal processes, and our understanding of their neural implementation, remain in their infancy.

Our lab takes inspiration from the observation that insects, despite having tiny brains, appear capable of performing specific cognitive operations extremely well. For example, desert ants know if they are 30 or 90 m away from the nest and honeybees communicate, with the waggle dance, if a flower patch is 30˚ or 90˚ to the left of the sun. By seeking comprehensive descriptions of how insect brains perform such internal calculations, we aim to provide a framework for thinking more clearly about cognition in larger brains.

We study the genetically tractable fruit fly, Drosophila melanogaster, and we have developed one of the primary approaches that we use –– monitoring neuronal activity in flies performing locomotor (flight or walking) behaviors while glued to a tiny platform.

In one line of work, we have described how the fly brain constructs an internal sense of angular orientation. Humans too have a robust sense of orientation, and its importance is made clear when we become disoriented upon exiting a subway station, or in the early stages of Alzheimer’s disease. Specifically, we have characterized a neuronal circuit in the Drosophila central complex––a set of structures in the middle of the fly brain––that allows flies to build and update an internal compass-like signal. This circuit operates even in complete darkness, wherein flies must internally integrate how much their legs are turning to update their sense of angular heading. The central features of this biological circuit are analogous to models proposed for how rodents build an internal sense of orientation, and may thus inform how the mammalian brain performs mathematical integration.

A fly glued to a custom platform walks in place, rotating an air-supported ball underneath. The fly’s left and right turns are linked in closed-loop, with minimal latency, to the movements of a vertical blue bar on a panoramic LED display surrounding the fly. The blue bar simulates a fixed landmark located far away from the animal (like the sun or a distant mountain) which indicates the fly’s orientation in the virtual environment. Neural signals recorded in a structure called the protocerebral bridge are shown above the fly. The protocerebral bridge expresses two bumps of activity that shift left and right along the bridge in lock step with the fly’s clockwise and counterclockwise rotations of its body, serving as an internal, compass-like signal that indicates the fly’s angular orientation in the virtual world. Similar compass-like neural signals are used by many animals to guide spatial navigation.

In another line of work, we have discovered a mechanism that allows brains to add mathematical vectors. First, we found that flies calculate more than just their orientation in reference to external cues (described above). They also have neurons that explicitly signal the direction in which they are traveling (walking or flying) in reference to those same cues. A fly’s orientation and traveling direction differ when she walks backwards or sideways, for example. To calculate the traveling-direction in reference to external cues, we discovered that flies have a neural circuit in the central complex that rotates, scales, and adds four input vectors, which track the fly’s traveling direction referenced to their body axis. The principles of this neural circuit for simple vector math may generalize to other brains, and cognitive domains beyond navigation, where vector operations or reference-frame transformations are required.

Top left: A fly walks in a circle while facing north (up) the whole time. The instantaneous direction of travel (red vector) is decomposed into components (black vectors). Top right: the same trajectory decomposed into components along axes rotated by 45˚. Bottom right: The fan-shaped body (gray outline) includes four arrays of neurons, each expressing a sinusoidally shaped bump of activity (brown and orange curves). The amplitude of each brown and orange sinusoid represents the fly’s traveling direction projected onto one of the four axes shown at the top right. A fifth array of neurons sums the four sinusoids (red curve) and the position of this red sinusoid’s peak represents the fly’s traveling direction. Bottom left: the peak position of the red array of neurons indicates the fly’s traveling direction (red dot). A different array of neurons in the donut-shaped ellipsoid body express a bump of activity whose position tracks the fly’s orientation (blue dot). Since the fly is always oriented in the same direction (north), this bump does not move.

The lab continues to study how flies calculate quantitatively accurate, spatial and non-spatial variables, and use these to guide behavior. For example, we have ongoing projects on egg-laying substrate decisions and simple visual conditioning (schematics below). This research program provides a platform for discovering basic mechanisms of how brains integrate, think, and decide.